One fish, two fish

Research shows lots of animals can count, even teeny ones: It doesn’t require a great big brain

Arkansas Democrat-Gazette Fish Illustration
Arkansas Democrat-Gazette Fish Illustration

Every night during breeding season, the male tungara frog of Central America will stake out a performance patch in the local pond and spend unbroken hours broadcasting male splendor to the world.

The mud-brown frog is barely the size of a shelled pecan, but its call is large and dynamic, a long, downward sweep that sounds remarkably like a phaser weapon on Star Trek, followed by a brief, twangy, harmonically dense chuck.

Unless, that is, a competing male starts calling nearby, in which case the first frog is likely to add two chucks to the tail of his sweep. And should his rival respond likewise, Male A will tack on three chucks.

Back and forth they go, call and raise, until the frogs appear to hit their respiratory limit at six to seven rapid-fire chucks.

The acoustic one-upfrogship is energetically draining and risks attracting predators such as bats. Yet the male frogs have no choice but to keep count of the competition, for the simple reason that female tungaras are doing the same: listening, counting and ultimately mating with the male of maximum chucks.

Behind the frog's surprisingly sophisticated number sense, scientists have found, are specialized cells in the amphibian midbrain that tally up sound signals and the intervals between them.

"The neurons are counting the number of appropriately timed pulses, and they're highly selective," said Gary Rose, a biologist at the University of Utah. If the timing between pulses is off by just a fraction of a second, the neurons don't fire and the counting process breaks down.

"It's Game Over," Rose said. "Just as in human communication, an inappropriate comment can end the whole conversation."

The story of the frog's neuro-abacus is one example of nature's vast, ancient and versatile number sense, a talent explored in detail in a recent theme issue of the journal Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, edited by Brian Butterworth, a cognitive neuroscientist at University College London, C. Randy Gallistel of Rutgers University and Giorgio Vallortigara of the University of Trento in Italy.

Scientists have found that animals across the evolutionary spectrum have a keen sense of quantity, able to distinguish not just bigger from smaller or more from less, but two from four, four from 10, 40 from 60.

Orb-weaving spiders, for example, keep a tally of how many silk-wrapped prey items are stashed in the "larder" segment of their web. When scientists experimentally remove the cache, the spiders spend time searching for the stolen goods in proportion to how many separate items had been taken, rather than how big the total prey mass might have been.

Small fish benefit from living in schools, and the more numerous the group, the statistically better a fish's odds of escaping predation. As a result, many shoaling fish are excellent appraisers of relative head counts.

Guppies, for example, have a so-called contrast ratio of 0.8, which means they can distinguish at a glance between four guppies and five, or eight guppies and 10, and if given the chance will swim toward the slightly fishier crowd.

Three-spined sticklebacks are more discriminating still: With a contrast ratio of .86, they're able to tell six fellow fish from seven, or 18 from 21 -- a comparative power that many birds, mammals and even humans might find hard to beat.

BORN TO COUNT

Despite the prevalence of math phobia, people also are born with a strong innate number sense, and numerosity is deeply embedded in many aspects of our minds and culture.

Researchers have determined that number words for small quantities -- less than five -- are strikingly similar across virtually every language group studied, and the words are among the most stable, unchanging utterances in any lexicon. They change less through time and across cultures than words for other presumably bedrock concepts like mother, father and most body parts, with a few puzzling exceptions such as the words for tongue and eye.

"The sounds that you and I use to say 'two' or 'three' are the sounds that have been used for tens of thousands of years," said Mark Pagel, a biologist at the University of Reading in Berkshire, England, who studies the evolution of language.

"It's not out of the question that you could have been wandering around 15,000 years ago and encountered a few of the last remaining Neanderthals, pointed to yourself and said, 'one,' and pointed to them and said, 'three,' and those words, in an odd, coarse way, would have been understood."

That continuity, Pagel added, "should astonish us."

ANCIENT BOOKKEEPERS

The earliest formal number systems, complete with those all-important place values, date only about 3,500 years, to ancient Mesopotamia, but quantitative record keeping is much older. Francesco D'Errico, an archaeologist at the University of Bordeaux, described his team's analysis of a 70,000-year-old hyena femur from the Les Pradelles site in France.

The leg bone fragment is incised with nine parallel, nearly identical notches, which look far too regular to be the incidental result of butchery with stone tools or an early attempt at artful decoration. "This fits with the idea that the incisions are a form of numerical notation," D'Errico said. "We see the beginnings of externalizing our numeric sense, of taking it outside the body."

Attitudes about animal numerosity have changed greatly since the mid-20th century when many researchers believed only humans had enough gray matter to think in numbers. They cited as an object lesson the 1907 case of Clever Hans, the horse that supposedly could solve arithmetic problems and would tap out his answers by hoof; as it turned out, he was responding to unconscious cues from the people around him.

Since then, researchers have approached the field with caution and rigor, seeking to identify specific evolutionary pressures that might spur the need for numeric judgments in any given species.

Social carnivores such as spotted hyenas, for example, live in fission-fusion societies, collectively defending their territories against rivals but in ever-shifting groups of widely roaming members. "You can never predict who you'll find in which group," said Sarah Benson-Amram, an assistant professor of zoology and physiology at the University of Wyoming. "You might be alone or in a group of 10."

Because hyena jaws can pulverize zebra bones, encounters between competitors can prove deadly; every spotted hyena must have a good sense from moment to moment of how many there are of us versus how many of them.

Benson-Amram and her colleagues tested hyena numerosity skills in the field, playing back the recorded whoops of spotted hyenas living in South Africa and Namibia to hyenas in Kenya. The Kenyan carnivores reacted to the whoops of strangers as predicted, approaching the hidden source of the sound when they had a home team advantage, retreating when they heard a few too many distinctive voices in a row.

Or sometimes, when the local hyenas were outnumbered, they'd call for backup.

"I always loved it when the hyenas would whoop and others would come running in," Benson-Amram said. "It was very dramatic, very aggressive, and I appreciated this ability to recruit other group members to the cause."

CHIMPS BEAT HUMANS

Chimpanzees are social scorekeepers, episodic warriors and number ninjas, too. They can be taught to associate groups of objects with corresponding Arabic numerals up to the number 9 and sometimes beyond -- three squares on a computer screen with the number 3, five squares with 5, and so on. They can put those numerals in order.

The numeric working memory of young chimpanzees is astonishing: Flash a random scattering of numerals on a screen for just 210 milliseconds -- half an eye blink -- and then cover the numbers with white squares, and a numerically schooled young chimpanzee will touch the squares sequentially to indicate the ascending order of the numbers hidden beneath.

Don't bother trying to do this yourself, Tetsuro Matsuzawa, a primatologist at Kyoto University, said at the scientific meeting in London on which the theme journal was based. "You can't."

By the look of it, cerebral property in humans once dedicated to numeric memory has been co-opted for grander purposes, such as the ability to judge whether a sentence like this is true: "There is no nonvanishing continuous tangent vector field on even dimensional spheres."

Stanislas Dehaene, a psychologist at the University of Paris, and his colleagues presented evidence that the neural circuitry for advanced mathematical thinking is an elaboration on the archaic number sense we share with other animals. They used brain scans of professional mathematicians.

It is distinct from our everyday language pathways, even when that math-related challenge involves words rather than numbers -- such as the true statement above.

Yet our inborn numerosity hardly guarantees mathematical proficiency, and can sometimes work against us. Psychologists Rochel Gelman of Rutgers University in New Jersey and Jennifer Jacobs Danan of the University of California, Los Angeles, have studied how often reasonably well-educated people miscalculate percentages.

We hear that the price of something rose by 50 percent and then fell by 50 percent, and we reflexively, mistakenly conclude, "Oh good, we're back to where we started." Our natural number sense adds and subtracts whole numbers, whole chucks, guppies and hyenas. But it really abhors a fraction, and so has led us astray.

photo

Photo courtesy BRIAN GRATWICKE

A male tungara frog (Engystomops pustulosus) inflates its bellows to call in this 2012 photo by Brian Gratwicke, a conservation biologist who leads the amphibian conservation programs at the Smithsonian Conservation Biology Institute, the Smithsonian National Zoo.

photo

The February issue of Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B was devoted to research into the origin of numerical abilities in animalsmand people.

ActiveStyle on 03/26/2018

Upcoming Events